Spooky result in aphantasia test
A new study shows people who cannot visualise mental images are harder to spook with scary stories.
Research on people with aphantasia – the inability to visualise mental images – tested how aphantasic people reacted to reading distressing scenarios, like being chased by a shark, falling off a cliff, or being in a plane that is about to crash.
The researchers physically measured each participant’s fear response by monitoring changing skin conductivity levels – or how much the story made a person sweat. Skin is known to become a better conductor of electricity when a person feels strong emotions, like fear.
According to the findings, scary stories lost their fear factor when the readers couldn’t visually imagine the scene – suggesting imagery may have a closer link to emotions than scientists previously thought.
“We found the strongest evidence yet that mental imagery plays a key role in linking thoughts and emotions,” says Professor Joel Pearson, senior author on the new paper.
“In all of our research to date, this is by far the biggest difference we’ve found between people with aphantasia and the general population.”
The researchers placed 46 study participants (22 with aphantasia, and 24 with imagery) in a blackened room and attached electrodes to their skin, before leaving the room and turning the light off.
The participants were left alone as a story started to appear on the screen in front of them.
At first, the stories started innocuously – for example, ‘You are at the beach, in the water’ or ‘You’re on a plane, by the window’. But as the stories continued, the suspense slowly built, whether it was a dark flash in the distant waves and people on the beach pointing, or the cabin lights dimming as the plane starts to shake.
“Skin conductivity levels quickly started to grow for people who were able to visualise the stories,” says Prof Pearson.
“The more the stories went on, the more their skin reacted.
“But for people with aphantasia, the skin conductivity levels pretty much flatlined.”
To check that differences in fear thresholds didn’t cause the response, the experiment was repeated using a series of scary images instead of text, like a photo of a cadaver or a snake baring its fangs.
This time, the pictures made the skin crawl equally in both groups of people.
“These two sets of results suggest that aphantasia isn’t linked to reduced emotion in general, but is specific to participants reading scary stories,” says Prof Pearson.
“The emotional fear response was present when participants actually saw the scary material play out in front of them.
“The findings suggest that imagery is an emotional thought amplifier. We can think all kind of things, but without imagery, the thoughts aren’t going to have that emotional ‘boom’.”
The idea for this experiment came after the research team noticed a recurring sentiment on aphantasia discussion boards that many people with the condition didn’t enjoy reading fiction.
While the findings suggest that reading may not be as emotionally impactful for people with aphantasia, Prof Pearson says it is important to note that the findings are based on averages, and not everyone with aphantasia will have the same reading experience.
The study was also focused on fear, and other emotional responses to fiction could be different.
“Aphantasia comes in different shapes and sizes,” he says.
“Some people have no visual imagery, while other people have no imagery in one or all of their other senses. Some people dream while others don’t.
“So don’t be concerned if you have aphantasia and don't fit this mould. There are all kinds of variations to aphantasia that we’re only just discovering.”